교회안내
Church & Ecclesia

예배와 말씀
Worship & Kerygma

훈련과 섬김
Didache & Diakonia

다음세대&다리사역
Youth/EM

교제와 선교
Koinonia & Diaspora

다음세대&다리사역
Next Generation & Bridge Ministry
꿈나무사역
Infant & Sunday School
유스그룹
Youth Group
대학청년부
Collge & Young Adult
2세 뿌리교육
Korean School
다리사역
Bridge Ministry
성경백과
Bible Encyclopedia
       
다리사역 Bridge Ministry

diffusion of innovation theory


“혁신의 확산 법칙” (diffusion of innovation theory)

2.5% Innovators 를 지난 후, 13.5% Early Adopters 를 지나야만 68%의 선도적 대중과 후발 대중이 따라온다는 법칙(Glenbrook UMC)

 


Diffusion of Innovation Theory

Diffusion research examines how ideas are spread among groups of people.  Diffusion goes beyond the two-step flow theory, centering on the conditions that increase or decrease the likelihood that an innovation, a new idea, product or practice, will be adopted by members of a given culture.  In multi-step diffusion, the opinion leader still exerts a large influence on the behavior of individuals, called adopters, but there are also other intermediaries between the media and the audience’s decision-making.  One intermediary is the change agent, someone who encourages an opinion leader to adopt or reject an innovation (Infante, Rancer, & Womack, 1997).

Innovations are not adopted by all individuals in a social system at the same time.  Instead, they tend to adopt in a time sequence, and can be classified into adopter categories based upon how long it takes for them to begin using the new idea.  Practically speaking, it’s very useful for a change agent to be able to identify which category certain individuals belong to, since the short-term goal of most change agents is to facilitate the adoption of an innovation.  Adoption of a new idea is caused by human interaction through interpersonal networks.  If the initial adopter of an innovation discusses it with two members of a given social system, and these two become adopters who pass the innovation along to two peers, and so on, the resulting distribution follows a binomial expansion.  Expect adopter distributions to follow a bell-shaped curve over time (Rogers, 1971). 

Adopter Categorization
The criterion for adopter categorization is innovativeness.  This is defined as the degree to which an individual is relatively early in adopting a new idea then other members of a social system.  Innovativeness is considered “relative” in that an individual has either more or less of it than others in a social system (Rogers, 1971). 

 Fig. 1 – Adopter categorization on the basis of innovativeness

Adopter distributions closely approach normality.  The above figure shows the normal frequency distributions divided into five categories: innovators, early adopters, early majority, late majority and laggards.  Innovators are the first 2.5 percent of a group to adopt a new idea.  The next 13.5 percent to adopt an innovation are labeled early adopters.  The next 34 percent of the adopters are called the early majority.  The 34 percent of the group to the right of the mean are the late majority, and the last 16 percent are considered laggards (Rogers, 1971).

The above method of classifying adopters is not symmetrical, nor is it necessary for it to be so.  There are three categories to the left of the mean and only two to the right.  While it is possible to break the laggard group into early and late laggards, research shows this single group to be fairly homogenous.  While innovators and early adopters could be combined, research shows these two groups as having distinctly different characteristics.  The categories are 1) exhaustive, in that they include all units of study, 2) mutually exclusive, excluding from any other category a unit of study already appearing in a category, and 3) derived from one classificatory principle.  This method of adopter categorization is presently the most widely used in diffusion research (Rogers, 1971).

Adopter Categories
Innovators are eager to try new ideas, to the point where their venturesomeness almost becomes an obsession. Innovators’ interest in new ideas leads them out of a local circle of peers and into social relationships more cosmopolite than normal.  Usually, innovators have substantial financial resources, and the ability to understand and apply complex technical knowledge.  While others may consider the innovator to be rash or daring, it is the hazardous risk-taking that is of salient value to this type of individual.  The innovator is also willing to accept the occasional setback when new ideas prove unsuccessful (Rogers, 1971).

Early adopters tend to be integrated into the local social system more than innovators.  The early adopters are considered to be localites, versus the cosmopolite innovators.  People in the early adopter category seem to have the greatest degree of opinion leadership in most social systems.  They provide advice and information sought by other adopters about an innovation.  Change agents will seek out early adopters to help speed the diffusion process.  The early adopter is usually respected by his or her peers and has a reputation for successful and discrete use of new ideas (Rogers, 1971). 

Members of the early majority category will adopt new ideas just before the average member of a social system.  They interact frequently with peers, but are not often found holding leadership positions.  As the link between very early adopters and people late to adopt, early majority adopters play an important part in the diffusion process.  Their innovation-decision time is relatively longer than innovators and early adopters, since they deliberate some time before completely adopting a new idea.  Seldom leading, early majority adopters willingly follow in adopting innovations (Rogers, 1971).

The late majority are a skeptical group, adopting new ideas just after the average member of a social system.  Their adoption may be borne out of economic necessity and in response to increasing social pressure.  They are cautious about innovations, and are reluctant to adopt until most others in their social system do so first.  An innovation must definitely have the weight of system norms behind it to convince the late majority.  While they may be persuaded about the utility of an innovation, there must be strong pressure from peers to adopt (Rogers, 1971).

Laggards are traditionalists and the last to adopt an innovation.  Possessing almost no opinion leadership, laggards are localite to the point of being isolates compared to the other adopter categories.  They are fixated on the past, and all decisions must be made in terms of previous  generations.  Individual laggards mainly interact with other traditionalists.  An innovation finally adopted by a laggard may already be rendered obsolete by more recent ideas already in use by innovators.   Laggards are likely to be suspicious not only of innovations, but of innovators and change agents as well (Rogers, 1971). 

Uses and Gratification 

Uses and gratification is more a concept of research than a self-contained theory.  Even contributors in this field of research find problems with the scope of the research and call uses and gratification an umbrella concept in which several theories reside (Infante et al. 1997). Researchers in this field argue that scholars have tried to do too much and should limit the scope and take a cultural-empirical approach to how people choose from the abundance of cultural products available. 

Critics claim the theory pays too much attention to the individual and does not look at the social context and the role the media plays in that social context. Rubin (1985), as cited in Littlejohn (1996), suggests that audience motive research based on uses and gratification research has been too compartmentalized within certain cultures and demographic groups, leading to the assumption this has thwarted synthesis and integration of research results, which are two key ingredients in theory building. 

The uses and gratification theory is a basic extension of the definition of an attitude, which is a non-linear cluster of beliefs, evaluations, and perceptions.  These beliefs, evaluations, and perceptions give individuals latitude over how they employ media in their lives; in other words, how individuals filter, interpret, and convey to others the information received from a medium.  Basically, a person’s attitude toward a segment of the media is determined by beliefs about and evaluations of the media.  A key to this research is that the consumer, or audience member, is the focal point instead of the message.  The research views the members of an audience as actively utilizing media contents, rather than being passively acted upon by the media, according to Katz, Blumer, and Gurevitch (1971) as cited in Littlejohn (1996).  When audience members, not the media, are the action takers, the variations taken from the messages received are the intervening variables.

A core assumption of uses and gratification research is the assumption that individual needs are satisfied by audience members actively seeking out the mass media (Infante et al., 1997).  Rubin (1983), as cited in Littlejohn (1996), designed a study to explore adult viewers’ motivations, behaviors, attitudes and patterns of interaction to see if behavioral and attitudinal consequences of the viewer could be predicted.  In 1984, the researcher identified two types of television viewers.  The first type is the habitual viewer who watches television for a diversion, has a high regard for the medium, and is a frequent user  The second type is the non-habitual viewer who is selective, likes a particular program or type of programs and uses the medium primarily for information.  The non-habitual viewer is more goal oriented when watching television and does not necessarily feel that television is important.   Rubin (1983) argues that habitual viewers use the medium as a companion and that non-habitual viewers are more actively involved  in the viewing experience (Littlejohn, 1996).

Expectancy-value theory 
Another theory to consider under this umbrella of uses and gratification research is expectancy-value theory from information-integration theorist Martin Fishbein (Littlejohn, 1996).   The researcher proposes there are two kinds of belief; belief in something and belief about something.  The example used by Fishbein is the person who believes in marijuana as a recreational drug or the person who believes that using marijuana will move on to other drugs and serious crimes in order to continue the habit. 

In Fishbein’s theory development, attitudes are different from beliefs in that they are evaluative and are correlated with beliefs and predispose a person to behave a certain way toward the attitude object.  The two beliefs about marijuana mentioned above would change dramatically if more serious drugs and crime were evaluated as bad. Also cited in Littlejohn is Philip Palmgreen, an early uses and gratification researcher, who claims that gratifications are sought in terms of a person’s beliefs about what a medium can provide and that person’s evaluation of the medium’s content (Littlejohn, 1996).

 

This entry was posted in 다리사역 Bridge Ministry, 자유게시판 Free Board. Bookmark the permalink.

Comments are closed.

No.TitleWriterDateHit
152 제 181차 워싱턴 통일광장기도회(장재웅목사) webmaster 2024.09.16 77
151 세계 명문대가 뽑고싶은 인재 webmaster 2024.09.06 82
150 2024 fresh conference webmaster 2024.08.11 107
149 뉴송처치(New Song) webmaster 2024.08.05 89
148 매리너스교회 webmaster 2024.07.31 151
147 모퉁이돌 교회 탐방(한국) webmaster 2024.07.30 89
146 움직이는 교회-개척 2년만에 100명의 성도(20-40대)가 모이는 교회 webmaster 2024.07.30 107
145 중국 주롱교회(Julong Church) webmaster 2024.07.01 136
144 테마예배를 드리는 상가교회-하늘샘교회(KMC) webmaster 2024.05.24 164
143 메타버스 다음세대교회(Metabus Church) webmaster 2024.05.06 188
< Prev 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 16 ... Next > 
 
    워싱턴 하늘비전교회 Heavenly Vision Community Church | 담임목사: 장재웅 (Rev. Jaewoong Chang)
Address:7565 Teague Rd Hanover MD 21076 | Cell: (410)200-3859 | Office: (410)595-6958 | Email: mdkumc@gmail.com
COPYRIGHT © 2018 Heavenly Vision Community Church. All Rights Reserved.
Powered by Heavenly Vision Community Church